1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 364 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

8. Evolution, Ecology and Behavior: The Expression of Variation: Reaction Norms


Poziom:

Temat: Nauka i technologia

Prof: Today we're going to talk about developmental
plasticity and reaction norms, and in the process we are going
to complete our assemblage of all of the tools we need to
understand microevolution, at least as a first sketch.
You'll recall that last time we were discussing developmental
control genes and the way they lay down basic patterns in body
plans.
They provide insight into the deep history of developmental
constraint and phylogenetic constraint,
and they also set up patterns that then interact,
during the course of development of individual
organisms, interact with the environment
to determine what the phenotype actually looks like.
The main thing that I'll be talking about today is the
concept of a reaction norm, and in so doing I would like to
fundamentally alter the way that you probably think about
organisms.
I want you to think about organisms, or about genomes at
least, as having the potential to produce many different
things.
The actual thing that is realized depends upon the
particular environments encountered,
the particular history of that individual organism,
and this can have profound effects on the way it looks,
the way it behaves, and how long it lives.
So this completes our basic understanding of all of the
fundamental processes that are operating in microevolution.
And after this point we're going to go on to discuss major
features of phenotypes, and on Monday we'll discuss the
evolution of sex, and we'll go on to discuss
things like life history evolution,
sex allocation, and genetic conflict:
all of those sorts of things.
So today I'm going to define a reaction norm.
I'll tell you where it fits in the evolutionary process;
where it came from; how it interacts with genetics;
how you can actually visualize the simultaneous effects of
genes and environment by making reaction norm plots.
That's an important thing.
There's been a long controversy in our general culture about
Nature versus Nurture.
Today I'm giving you the tool to take that issue apart and
understand it rigorously.
You will end up seeing that all aspects of all organisms are
determined both by genes and by environment, and there are clear
ways to think about it.
Then I'll show you how this kind of immediate,
short-term phenotypic plasticity interacts with
developmental control genes and phylogenetic constraints,
and I'll do that with the butterfly wing,
and we will see at the end that in fact biology is--
heh, it's not surprising--biology is complexly
organic, in a very deep way,
and we can see that in the butterfly wing example.
So, this is what a reaction norm is.
Okay?
It's a property of a genotype.
One can also define reaction norms for larger collections of
things.
You can define a reaction norm for a family,
for example.
All the sibs in a family might share a certain component of
their reaction to the environment.
But strictly speaking, a reaction norm is just the
property of a single genotype.
So what it does is describe the set of phenotypes into which one
genotype can be mapped, as the environment varies.
So in the simplest case you have one trait and you have one
environmental variable, and this is the way that that
genotype, one genotype,
would react to this one environmental variable.
Now, organisms have lots of traits,
and there are lots of environmental variables,
and so you can immediately see that this simple picture can be
generalized into an N-dimensional reaction surface.
It can get very complex if we're not just dealing about
temperature, but say food,
population density, presence or absence of members
of the other sex, many things.
And we think about that happening over the whole course
of the organism's life; you can generate quite a
complex reaction surface.
So each genotype has the potential to end up anywhere
along this reaction surface, depending upon the
environmental history.
So the study of reaction norms is intended to make that process
explicit.
Now where's it fit?
In the last lecture I gave you one slide that had on it,
"This is what ecology and behavior do;
this is what genetics does; this is what development does,
in evolution."
I am now repeating that slide, that basic message,
in a diagrammatic context.
Okay?
So we can think of the evolutionary process basically
as being a cycle that moves between genotype space and
phenotype space.
So this is one generation, this is another generation.
And reaction norms develop at the stage where the genotypes
that are present in the just fertilized zygote are being
translated into the phenotypes of the adults.
When the gametes are then mapped into genotypes,
to produce this array here--so when the zygotes are
formed--this is the Hardy-Weinberg Law;
this is basically population genetics up here.
Down here, when the phenotypes that are being produced by the
reaction norms then undergo behavior and ecology to
determine a surviving set of organisms that can mate and
reproduce and have babies, that's natural selection,
down here.
Now the important thing is that all of these things go on in
every generation.
You can't get away from any of them.
In every generation there's genetics, in every generation
there's development, and in every generation there's
ecology and behavior.
So they're all necessary components of understanding the
microevolutionary process.
This is the first picture ever made of a reaction norm.
It was done by a German guy named Woltereck,
working in lakes near Munich, and so he called them
reaktionsnormen, not reaction norms.
And what you see here are the morphological changes that are
going on between generations-- so this is a mother,
this is her offspring, this is the offspring of this
one, and so forth-- within a single clone.
These are water fleas that are reproducing asexually.
So what you're looking at is a series of different phenotypes
that are all produced by the same genotype,
the genotype is being copied exactly,
and in the middle of the summer they are producing these helmets
and spines.
There are a number of cases that are pretty well studied
where this happens.
There are spines, helmets and neck teeth in these
water fleas, which are called Daphnia,
and they are induced by dissolved molecules that are
associated with predators, and the predator's efficiency
in eating those Daphnia is affected by the production of
those spines and helmets, on the Daphnia.
Making a spine or a helmet has a reproductive cost.
So if the predator's not around, you don't want to make
the spine, because it's costing you babies.
So it is a contingent plastic reaction.
You get a signal from the environment that says,
"Oh, oh, danger.
What do I do?"
Well basically what you do is you modify the development of
your offspring so that they're safer,
but your offspring won't be able to have as many babies
because they're better at not being eaten.
There are bent shells in barnacles that do the same
thing; they make them resistant to
snail predators, but they reduce the barnacle's
fecundity.
This cost is important.
If the cost were not there, then the organisms would make
the defensive structure all the time.
If it was cost-free, why not do it all the time?
Okay?
But it's not, it cost them something,
and so they're forced to compromise,
and they try to minimize the cost of the defensive structures
by not producing them unless they get a signal that there's
danger.
Snails parasitized by castrating digenetic trematodes
reproduce earlier.
By the way, this digenetic trematode is also called
schistosomiasis.
So this snail is an intermediate host for a serious
human disease.
Let's take a look at these.
When Daphnia smell midge larvae, in the water--
a midge larva is a little invertebrate predator that swims
around and it catches Daphnia with its fore legs,
like this, and if Daphnia makes a little neck tooth,
it makes it harder for the midge larvae to handle it.
Evidently this neck tooth actually,
although it looks very small, cost Daphnia something,
because they only make it when they smell midge larvae in the
water.
This is a modern photograph of the helmet and the tail spine on
Daphnia.
You can see they're really quite dramatic.
And this is where the cost is borne, here.
You can see the number of eggs being produced,
and there are fewer eggs in the body of this Daphnia than there
would be in a mature member of this one.
This one has actually just given birth, so its eggs are all
out of its body.
In barnacles this is what it looks like.
If the barnacle smells the snails, when it's growing up,
it grows up in a clumped-over form.
It bends as it grows, and instead of feeding freely
out of the top of its body, it feeds very inefficiently,
and it pays a price in not being able to make more babies.
Barnacles, by the way, are essentially shrimp that
swim around as larvae and glue themselves to the substrate and
spend their lives stuck to the basement,
kicking food into their mouth with their feet.
So the feet of the barnacle would be sticking out here.
Charles Darwin spent seven years working on barnacles,
figuring out that they were actually crustaceans.
This is the data on schistosomiasis,
and the neat thing about this experiment is that the reaction
in the snail is induced just by water in which there have been
parasites, not by the parasites themselves.
In other words, you just give the snail a
whiff, a little bit of scent that a
parasite is likely to get into its body,
and its reaction is, "Oh gosh,
I am going to die from a parasite, so I better start
reproducing."
So it shifts its reproduction earlier in life;
and you can see the extent of that shift right here.
These are the ones that have been exposed to water with
parasites in it; these are the unexposed
controls.
So these things that I'm describing are all induced
responses, they are all plastic reactions
to signals in the environment, and they all shape the reaction
norms of these organisms.
So those are some concrete cases.
Now let's look at sort of the abstract, visual,
analytic framework a little bit.
Here is one reaction norm.
I have sketched a common one for many poikilothermic
organisms; many things that are--you would
think of them as cold-blooded; they don't regulate their body
temperature.
The higher the temperature, the smaller they are at
maturity.
That would work--this general relationship describes how
tadpoles grow, how many fish grow.
If you look at a population, it can be conceived of as a
bundle of reaction norms.
So there are many genotypes out there.
So if we have a very small population with just about five
or six genotypes in it-- the green dotted lines are the
individual reaction norms for the different genotypes,
and there could be a population mean reaction norm.
You just calculate the mean value across all the
environments and all the genotypes, and that describes
how that population responds.
This is important when you're trying to summarize this kind of
complexity in ecology.
You want to know how one population might react as the
environment changes, so that you can analyze its
impact on another one: a predator acting on a prey;
a parasite acting on a host; a grazer acting on a plant.
This is a good picture to have in your mind of what that
population looks like.
Now traits can have very different expression patterns;
it's not as though all traits have very dramatic reaction
norms.
And I just chose the example of five digits in many tetrapods,
including ourselves, to indicate that you could have
three different genotypes, and you could change population
density a lot, and the number of digits on the
hand wouldn't change.
Everybody would have five fingers.
There are some things that are just not sensitive to the
environment.
Okay?
So think of the individual organism as a mosaic of
sensitivities.
Some of it is not sensitive at all to changes in the
environment, or almost insensitive, and other parts of
it are quite sensitive.
For example, fecundity.
If you increase population density, fecundity will go down
in individual organisms, because they're having to
compete harder to get food.
So if you restrict food, by any mechanism,
fecundity will drop--and an increase in population density
is one way to do it-- and the genotypes in the
population can react differently to that increase.
In all three cases fecundity decreased, but genotype 1 was
quite sensitive, and genotype 3 was much less
sensitive to the shift in population density;
and that makes a difference.
As a matter of fact, if you think about it,
right here, if you have a fluctuating population,
and this population is going between low density and high
density, you have a method of
maintaining genetic variation right there,
because the reaction norms cross, and the guys that were
good at one density are lousy at the other.
So if the population cycles back and forth between them,
one time G1 is favored, the next time G3 is favored,
and so forth.
Okay?
So I'm trying to develop the notion that by sketching
reaction norms, you can come up,
very quickly, with a useful analytical
picture of what's going on in a population.
For example, if you have this sort of a
reaction norm pattern for four genotypes,
and you select upward here, you're going to lead to no
response over here at all, because they all happen to
converge at this point.
So selection here doesn't make any difference to what you
observe in this part of the environment.
But in this case, the crossing reaction norm case
that we had in the last picture with fecundity,
if you select upward in this environment,
you're going to have a downward response here.
If we select at low population density,
and population density is low for a long time,
it's going to produce a shift in the population over here,
because G1 will be favored, and it has low fecundity at
high density.
Okay?
That's this situation.
We can just look at a sketch of a reaction norm and we get a
sense for how sensitive that trait is to changes in the
environment.
This is not a very plastic trait, it's pretty insensitive,
and we can see that because it has a shallow slope.
This trait's very sensitive.
You change the environment a little, it changes a lot.
Now it's not just spines and helmets that have reaction
norms.
This is a picture of an Affymetrix GeneChip for
Drosophila melanogaster-- it's got 13,500 genes--and what
the chip is doing is it's picking up the messenger RNA,
which is being expressed in the organism;
and the intensity of light that you see at a given spot is a
measure of the concentration of messenger RNA for that
particular gene.
So in one picture you have a summary view of the output of
the entire genome.
Okay?
These things have reaction norms.
I put this in for Andrea.
Okay?
Andrea just wrote a paper about this.
So these things have reaction norms.
If I gave you Drosophila and I exposed them to high temperature
and low temperature and you extracted their mRNA and you ran
them out on a GeneChip and you compared the two patterns,
you would see big differences in the patterns of all of those
light spots.
And if you did that carefully, you would be able to draw the
reactions of the expression patterns for all the 13,500
genes in the genome.
So these concepts are general.
They're not limited to morphology.
They apply to any aspect of the phenotype, and this is now a
very popular way to measure phenotypes.
Okay?
There are lots of things like GeneChips out there.
How many people in the audience know, or have heard of
GeneChips, or other methods of measuring outputs?
You're not quite densely scattered enough to have you
turn to everyone around you and explain what they are.
>
If I had about twice the density, I could just stop
talking and have you all explain to each other what a GeneChip
is.
Okay.
We can leave that for a later date.
Suffice it to say that in modern molecular technology
these things, which are now just about ten
years old, a little over ten years old,
are methods of looking at the expression of all the genes in
the genome all at once; and they too have reaction
norms.
So to sum up on reaction norms.
A reaction norm is a description of how genes are
mapped into the phenotype as a function of the environment.
They are properties of genotypes.
So if you really want a proper, rigorous way of measuring a
reaction norm, you have to be able to clone
the organism, so you can get the same
genotype replicated and then test it in different
environments.
If you wanted to do that for humans, what kind of data would
you use?
Student: Twins.
Prof: Twins.
What kind?
Student: Identical.
Prof: Identical twins.
Identical twins are probably the only--I suppose there might
rarely be, these days, identical triplets.
I suppose there might even be, somewhere in California,
identical octuplets.
But most of the time we deal with identical twins,
and that's about as far as you can go, in humans,
with this sort of thing.
But in Daphnia, or in plants,
it's possible to get genotypes replicated,
up to a hundred individuals sometimes,
and then you can make a very accurate measure of a reaction
norm.
You can think of a population as a bundle of individual
reaction norms; and that's an important concept
because when we come to ecology we're going to be thinking about
how predators interact with prey,
and about how competitors interact with each other.
And when we do that, normally the way that
biologists have done it in the past is they've chunked those
things as species, where they have a species
typical property.
Okay?
So all the species 1 are supposed to behave one way,
and all of species 2 are supposed to behave another way.
But the differences between the individuals in those species are
really important, and when the two species are
interacting, it's not like they're all
identical individuals interacting.
They are different, and when the species interact
it's bundles of reaction norms interacting with bundles of
reaction norms.
And this produces important effects.
For example, it tends to stabilize
ecological interactions.
So remember that for say about six or eight weeks down the
line, when we get to ecology.
This property of populations has important consequences.
There's a real easy way to talk about the sensitivity of
phenotypes to the environment.
You just make a reaction norm plot and look at the slope.
If the slope is steep, those organisms are very
sensitive to changes in environment;
if it's flat, they are not.
And in terms of the kinds of intellectual tools that one
might pick up in the course of a liberal arts education,
in order to deal in later life with the claims of people who
want to talk about the evolution of IQ,
or racial differences, or lots of stuff that involves
assumptions about genetic determination,
reaction norms are useful because they visually describe
the contributions of genes and environment to the phenotype.
And, for example, I will put up a speculative
plot, just to illustrate the potential social significance of
what I'm talking about.
If, for example, I put IQ up here and I put
Family Annual Income down here--already we're in trouble,
right; we're not being politically
correct anymore-- and then I do this,
basically what I'm saying is that if I took human identical
twins and I raised one here and the other one here,
I could get that.
Okay?
And what that shows you--by the way, I don't know that that is
true; I'm just trying to give you
something to remember, that will convince you that
this sort of analysis can potentially be significant--
what that shows you basically is that people might appear to
be real smart in one environment and stupid in another,
compared to the other ones in the populations,
and that these things are context dependent.
So, that's just an illustration of this point down here on the
bottom.
Okay, so I've been talking a lot about phenotypic plasticity,
and I've shown you these wonderful examples of Daphnia
reacting sensitively to predators and so forth.
Does that mean that organisms are really plastic?
Can I just pick up a bunch of clay and mold it into anything
that I want, depending on the environment that I expose it to?
No I can't.
And that's because, as we learned last time,
the large-scale structure is determined by things that are
hard to change, and those are developmental
patterns that have a deep evolutionary history,
and they set up a rigid framework within which the
plasticity is expressed.
So the things that change slowly--those are the
developmental control genes--are constraining the things that
change rapidly.
I just lost a little bit of text off the bottom.
So let's do this with the example of Distal-less.
Distal-less is a developmental control gene.
The pictures here basically are showing you how the Drosophila
larva gets set up very early in development.
The first thing that happens is that an anterior/posterior axis
gets laid down.
That's done by the Hox genes.
Then the dorsoventral axis is determined by Sog and Chordin
and Decapentaplegic and things like that.
Then, after the basic axes of the organisms are laid down and
segments are formed, other things turn on that
determine whether you'll be dealing with a head,
a gut or a tail.
Interestingly, the name for the gene that
induces heart formation is Tinman, from the Wizard of Oz,
who didn't have a heart.
Okay?
So they give neat names to some of these things.
And what we're worried about today is this gene here,
Distal-less, which determines body wall
outgrowth.
Remember last time I also showed you that picture of Pax6;
that's the gene that induces eye formation.
But today we're going to talk about Distal-less.
And if you look at the body of a fly, this is where the action
of certain mutations takes place.
If you get mutations in Distal-less, these are the parts
of the body which are going to be affected.
They are all extremities, all out-pocketings of the body
wall, which are then being developed into antennae or mouth
parts or legs.
Vestigial is working on wings and haltiers,
and Eyeless is working on the presence of eyes.
Okay?
Now in order to tell you about this deep developmental
constraint in butterfly wings, I first want you to notice that
there's something that's called a Nymphalid groundplan.
The Nymphalidae are a large family of butterflies,
and in the nineteenth century German biologists,
with German thoroughness, out to eight decimal places,
did an exhaustive study of thousands of butterfly wings,
and they were able to take that whole family of Nymphalidae,
with its hundreds of species, and reduce them all to
variations on these themes.
So they found that in the middle of the wing you could
have stripes; in the outer part of the wing
you could have what they called border eyespots,
or border ocelli; right on the edge of the wing
you could have bands, and so forth.
So that this would describe all of the different kinds of things
that you could do with butterflies.
And we're going to focus on the eyespots.
Now this is the diversity of butterfly wing patterns that you
can get in about ten minutes in the Peabody Museum collections.
They are beautiful, they're just amazing.
I remember the first time I saw a birdwing butterfly in the
collections at the Bishop Museum in Honolulu.
The birdwings come from New Guinea and other parts of
Southeast Asia.
They're about that big.
They're the largest butterflies on the planet.
And actually their form is a bit like this guy,
except they're about four times bigger.
And you can see that simply by varying the location where
colors are expressed, and by varying the size of the
different elements, you generate a huge number of
patterns.
You can even use them to write numbers on wings.
Evolution has written numbers on the back wing of this
particular butterfly; this is an '89 butterfly.
The model system in which this is best studied is in a
butterfly called Bicyclus, and it has been worked on by
Paul Brakefield in Leiden, and Sean Carroll in Madison,
Wisconsin, and Antonia Monteiro in our
department, and a number of other people,
Vern French in Edinburgh.
And Bicyclus has a number of neat features.
One of them is that it is developmentally plastic.
In the wet season it looks like this, and in the dry season it
looks like this.
And, in fact, these are two brothers who have
been produced in the laboratory, with this one being raised
under wet season conditions and this one being raised under dry
season conditions.
So one genotype can elicit a range of phenotypes,
and you can see that in the process the eyespots change
considerably in their size and intensity.
Now it turns out that you can fish the Distal-less gene out of
Drosophila, and you can use that segment of
DNA to recognize the homolog gene in the butterfly,
and you can then put a reporter onto the homolog,
and you can ask that gene to express its reporter when it's
being expressed, so that you can see visually
where the gene's being expressed.
When that's done, you can see that every place
that an eyespot is going to form in the adult wing,
you can see the gene being expressed in the wing disc,
in the developing pupa.
The way that butterflies and flies and other holometabolous
insects develop is that after the caterpillar or the larva has
fed for awhile, and it's starting to form its
pupa, the cells reorganize in the
pupa, into structures that are going to be parts of the adult,
and the wing disc, that's going to be the wing in
the adult, looks like this in the pupa,
and it's sitting right on the surface of the pupa.
So that if you want to do developmental biology
experiments on it, you can go through the wall of
the pupal case and you can pick out a few cells and you can move
them around.
So, in fact, you can go in and cut one of
these things out and put it down somewhere else;
if you do, it will make an eyespot there.
So this is actually an exceedingly neat system to work
in because you can actually do cell manipulations,
as well as genetic manipulations.
You can manipulate both the developmental biology and the
underlying genetic structure, in butterfly wings.
This is another species; it just has two eyespots,
and when you look at its wing disc, it just has two places
that Distal-less is being expressed;
and those are going to be right in the center,
right where that white spot is.
So Distal-less is actually telling the wing disc where to
make eyespots, and the Nymphalid groundplan
says you can only make those eyespots in certain places.
And the Nymphalid groundplan, the butterfly wing groundplan,
is arguably about 100,000,000 years old;
it's ancient.
So does that mean that you can't change the eyespots?
No it doesn't.
Almost everything about the eyespots has a reaction norm,
except their location and number.
Within a given species you're always going to get the same
number, and they're always going to be
in the same place, but whether they're big or so
small that you can't even see them depends on the environment
in which they're expressed.
So if you raise a whole bunch of families,
and you compare the siblings across families,
to make reaction norms, you can see that the diameter
of the white part of the spot and the diameter of the black
part of the spot changes as you go from low to high temperature.
You have low temperatures in the dry season and high
temperatures in the wet season, and that shifts the reaction
norms on the butterfly wing.
Well I'm a sucker for analogies, and analogies are
dangerous.
You might think that the eyespot was a vase,
and into that vase you're going to stick a bundle of reaction
norms.
And you can think of the vase as being the phylogenetic
history of the developmental constraint on the butterfly
wing, and it's holding those reaction
norms within a certain range, but that the environment then
is allowing them to vary, to the degree that a bundle of
flowers could flop out of a flower vase.
Well it turns out--I'm sorry for this;
this is something that I checked this morning and it
wasn't going on.
At any rate, I'll read this out for you.
Can we think of macroevolution as having constructed a vase,
within which the reaction norms sit?
And the answer is no.
And the answer is no because some of the genes that are
controlling the shape and the position of the eyespots--
so things like Distal-less--are also involved in determining the
slopes and the shapes of the reaction norms.
These two things are genetically entangled,
and their entanglement is a case of the same gene having two
different functions at different times in development,
and natural selection will operate on it throughout the
lifecycle.
So it's not as though there are some things that are
constraints, that are not being changed,
and there are other things that are genes that are sort of
tweaking the constraints a little bit.
In fact, the same genes are involved in producing both
things.
So if we want to shift the slope of the reaction norm by
selecting on phenotypic plasticity in Bicyclus,
we are going to be selecting on genes that are also determining
the location and number of eyespots.
If you think this kind of stuff is nice, you can go and look on
the Web, on these sorts of websites.
Antonia works on butterfly wing patterns.
Gunter works on the tetrapod limb, and with Vinny Lynch he
has recently been looking into the origin of the mammalian
female reproductive tract.
So they have been comparing things like duckbilled
platypuses and spiny echidnas-- which are mammals that lay
eggs--with kangaroos and eutherians--
which are mice and lions and things like us--
and discovering where it is that the mammalian female
reproductive tract actually came from.
It turns out that the HOX genes are involved in that,
and that it's another one of these stories of gene
duplication making the development of new structures
possible.
Rick Prum, who's our department chair and works in the Peabody
Museum, is one of the world experts on
feathers and on the fact that dinosaurs had feathers,
and if you're interested in working with Rick,
you can certainly drop in, and he's a very friendly guy
and would be happy to show you what he knows about feathers.
So this is an active area and it produces a lot of fascinating
research.
To summarize my overview of it, what I want to emphasize is
that the phenotype, the whole organism that you
see, and the whole lifecycle of that organism that you see,
is a mosaic of parts, and their pattern of
determination varies tremendously in evolutionary
age.
So if you just look at my own body,
the parts of me that are extremely old are the fact that
I have four limbs and five fingers,
and the parts of me that are evolutionarily relatively young
are the size of my cerebral cortex and some other aspects of
me.
And if you were to look into the plasticity of my cerebral
cortex, you would discover that it is
incredibly plastic, and that when I am a little
baby and I'm just born, I have billions more
connections in my nerve cells than I do when I'm
seven-years-old, and that a great deal of my
mental development, between birth and the age of
seven, has essentially been the
remodeling of my cortex by plastic interactions with the
environment.
And in fact that's what a lot of learning is about;
it's about plastic response to environment.
So I am myself, as are you, a mosaic of things,
of very different evolutionary ages.
The basic developmental patterns that we see in animals
are mostly about 500,000,000 years old.
In plants they're a bit younger.
The HOX control of body symmetry and body pattern in
animals is arguably about 600,000,000 years old;
maybe a little less, maybe 550,000,000.
The ABC pattern of flower development in flowering plants
is probably somewhere between about 95 and 135,000,000 years
old; that's something that happened
in the Cretaceous.
Now let's shift timescale and go down to one generation,
one organism, encountering a specific
environment.
Its plastic reaction to the environment has evolved
relatively recently, and it implements specific
contingency plans.
Daphnia that come from lakes that do not have fish in them
and haven't had fish in them for a long time,
don't react when you put the smell of a fish into the water.
The Daphnia that come from lakes that have had fish in them
for along time react, and react strongly and quickly.
So the plastic reaction is something that can evolve.
I want to caution you though, it is not as though all the
fine details of the plastic response are adaptive;
they are not necessarily all adaptive.
For example, think about temperature.
If we are studying the plastic reactions of organisms to
temperature, it may very well be that things
that live in the Arctic have a different reaction norm than
things that live in the tropics, because they've encountered a
different temperature regime, and that that's an evolved
reaction.
But it's also quite possible that it's just biophysically
impossible to do something when it gets colder;
that doesn't have to evolve.
So I want you also to be able to think of the necessity of
taking something like a plastic reaction norm and dissecting it
analytically so that you can figure out what part of it's
adaptive and what part of it is just there because that's the
kind of stuff that organisms are built out of.
They are biochemical systems, and biochemistry,
we know, has reaction rates that change with temperature and
with a lot of other things.
Okay?
So it's not--this is not all adaptive.
The thing you actually see, the organism you analyze,
is just one point on a multidimensional reaction
surface.
It could have been a lot of other things,
and all those other things that it could have been are important
when we think about evolutionary ecology,
when we think about population dynamics,
when we think about interactions between hosts and
parasites, because they represent all
those other potential interactions that could be going
on in other circumstances.
Okay?
So by thinking about reaction norms, we can both express the
genetic variation in the population;
we can express the developmental reaction to the
environment, the way all of those different genetic
combinations will react to the environment;
and we have the potential to visualize the dynamic over
generations, as both the gene frequencies and the
environmental circumstances change.
So there's a potential here for a lot of interesting analysis.
I think the basic take-home point though is this one.
Every phenotype is the product of both genetic and
environmental influences, and the way they interact to
produce the phenotype is extremely important.
So it is almost never the case that you can claim that only
Nature, or only Nurture, accounts for what you see in
organisms.
So that basically completes what I want you to know about
microevolutionary principles, before we now go into the
analysis of how natural selection shapes phenotypes for
reproductive success.
I'm going to use all these concepts.
For example, when we get to the evolution of
age of maturity, I'm going to talk about
reaction norms for age of maturity in human females,
and in fish, and in mammoths.
So I want you to remember these elements.
I also want you to remember, as we go forward,
that everything that you see in organisms has an evolutionary
history.
It doesn't have to be an adaptive history.
It might be drift.
Things might happen in phenotypes that are byproducts
of stuff that's going on somewhere else in the organism.
There are all kinds of alternatives that you should be
continually prepared to compare, when you're trying to analyze
what you see, but everything that you see has
evolved.
All you have to do to see that is remember at one point your
ancestors were bacteria, and everything else has come
since then.
So next time we're going to start talking about how
organisms are designed for reproductive success;
and our first step is why do they reproduce sexually?
Mobile Analytics