1. Po raz pierwszy odwiedzasz EDU. LEARN

    Odwiedzasz EDU.LEARN

    Najlepszym sposobem na naukę języka jest jego używanie. W EDU.LEARN znajdziesz interesujące teksty i videa, które dadzą Ci taką właśnie możliwość. Nie przejmuj się - nasze filmiki mają napisy, dzięki którym lepiej je zrozumiesz. Dodatkowo, po kliknięciu na każde słówko, otrzymasz jego tłumaczenie oraz prawidłową wymowę.

    Nie, dziękuję
  2. Mini lekcje

    Podczas nauki języka bardzo ważny jest kontekst. Zdjęcia, przykłady użycia, dialogi, nagrania dźwiękowe - wszystko to pomaga Ci zrozumieć i zapamiętać nowe słowa i wyrażenia. Dlatego stworzyliśmy Mini lekcje. Są to krótkie lekcje, zawierające kontekstowe slajdy, które zwiększą efektywność Twojej nauki. Są cztery typy Mini lekcji - Gramatyka, Dialogi, Słówka i Obrazki.

    Dalej
  3. Wideo

    Ćwicz język obcy oglądając ciekawe filmiki. Wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na filmik. Nie martw się, obok każdego z nich są napisy. A może wcale nie będą Ci one potrzebne? Spróbuj!

    Dalej
  4. Teksty

    Czytaj ciekawe artykuły, z których nauczysz się nowych słówek i dowiesz więcej o rzeczach, które Cię interesują. Podobnie jak z filmikami, możesz wybrać temat oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknąć na wybrany artykuł. Nasz interaktywny słownik pomoże Ci zrozumieć nawet trudne teksty, a kontekst ułatwi zapamiętanie słówek. Dodatkowo, każdy artykuł może być przeczytany na głos przez wirtualnego lektora, dzięki czemu ćwiczysz słuchanie i wymowę!

    Dalej
  5. Słowa

    Tutaj możesz znaleźć swoją listę "Moje słówka", czyli funkcję wyszukiwania słówek - a wkrótce także słownik tematyczny. Do listy "Moje słówka" możesz dodawać słowa z sekcji Videa i Teksty. Każde z słówek dodanych do listy możesz powtórzyć później w jednym z naszych ćwiczeń. Dodatkowo, zawsze możesz iść do swojej listy i sprawdzić znaczenie, wymowę oraz użycie słówka w zdaniu. Użyj naszej wyszukiwarki słówek w części "Słownictwo", aby znaleźć słowa w naszej bazie.

    Dalej
  6. Lista tekstów

    Ta lista tekstów pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Teksty". Wybierz poziom trudności oraz temat, a następnie artykuł, który Cię interesuje. Kiedy już zostaniesz do niego przekierowany, kliknij na "Play", jeśli chcesz, aby został on odczytany przez wirtualnego lektora. W ten sposób ćwiczysz umiejętność słuchania. Niektóre z tekstów są szczególnie interesujące - mają one odznakę w prawym górnym rogu. Koniecznie je przeczytaj!

    Dalej
  7. Lista Video

    Ta lista filmików pojawia się po kliknięciu na "Video". Podobnie jak w przypadku Tekstów, najpierw wybierz temat, który Cię interesuje oraz poziom trudności, a następnie kliknij na wybrane video. Te z odznaką w prawym górnym rogu są szczególnie interesujące - nie przegap ich!

    Dalej
  8. Dziękujemy za skorzystanie z przewodnika!

    Teraz już znasz wszystkie funkcje EDU.LEARN! Przygotowaliśmy do Ciebie wiele artykułów, filmików oraz mini lekcji - na pewno znajdziesz coś, co Cię zainteresuje!

    Teraz zapraszamy Cię do zarejestrowania się i odkrycia wszystkich możliwości portalu.

    Dziękuję, wrócę później
  9. Lista Pomocy

    Potrzebujesz z czymś pomocy? Sprawdź naszą listę poniżej:
    Nie, dziękuję

Już 62 368 użytkowników uczy się języków obcych z Edustation.

Możesz zarejestrować się już dziś i odebrać bonus w postaci 10 monet.

Jeżeli chcesz się dowiedzieć więcej o naszym portalu - kliknij tutaj

Jeszcze nie teraz

lub

Poziom:

Wszystkie

Nie masz konta?

5. Evolution, Ecology and Behavior: How Selection Changes the Genetic Composition of Population


Poziom:

Temat: Edukacja

Prof: Today we're going to talk about Adaptive Genetic
Change.
And in order to set the stage for this, before I get into the
slides, I would like you to consider the following
proposition.
Every evolutionary change on the planet,
that has ever led to something that you think is cool and
interesting and is well designed,
whether it is the brain of a bat, or the vertebrate immune
system, or the beautiful structure of
the ribosome, or the precision of meiosis,
has occurred through a process of adaptive genetic change.
A mutation has occurred that had an effect on a process or a
structure and, if it increased the
reproductive success of the organism that it was in,
it was retained by evolution; and if it did not,
it disappeared.
So what we're talking about today is a look into a very
basic mechanism that is operating in all of life and is
causing the accumulation of information.
Now, these are the keys to the lecture.
In the middle of the lecture you're going to get a couple of
slides that have tables and equations on them and stuff like
that, and I'll lead you through one
of those tables, and I'll ask you to go through
another one.
But they're not the point.
The point is this.
There are four major genetic systems, and there are some
interesting exceptions to them.
But you can capture a big chunk of the variation in the genetics
of the organisms on the planet with just four systems.
Okay?
They are sexual versus asexual and haploid versus diploid,
and those differences make a big difference to how fast
evolution occurs.
You guys are sexual diploids and you evolve slowly,
and your pathogens are asexual haploids and they evolve fast.
That's important, the kind of thing you ought to
know.
Now when we get into the equations of population
genetics-- they're just algebra--the point
is that you can always go find them in a book and you can
program them pretty easily, even in simple spreadsheet
programs like Excel, and you can understand their
basic properties by playing around with them.
If you go on the web and go to Google and type Hardy-Weinberg
equations, you're going to get 20 websites
around the country where some professor of population genetics
has put up some package for students to play with and it's
going to generate all kinds of beautiful pictures and stuff
like that.
It's real easy for you to lay your hands on these tools now.
What's important for you to know is (a) that they are there
and represent something important;
(b) what their major consequences are;
and (c) how to get a hold of them when you need them.
I am not going to ask you to repeat the derivation of the
Hardy-Weinberg equations on a mid-term.
Okay?
But I do expect you to know why they're important and what
they're about.
The third thing that I want you to take home from this lecture
is that when adaptive genetic change starts to occur,
it is virtually always slow at the beginning,
fast in the middle and slow at the end.
So that if you are looking at a graph of gene frequencies over
time, it looks like an S; and that's the third thing.
That's it, there's the lecture, ta-da.
Now background to this decision.
When, in 1993, Rolf Hoekstra and I began to
put together the first edition of this book,
I asked Rolf to be my co-author because he is a population
geneticist.
He has a marvelously clear mind.
He likes those kinds of equations and he's really good
at them.
And we, Rolf and I, went around and we asked about
fifteen of the leading evolutionary biologists in the
world, "What's important?
What should every biologist know about evolution?
This is for everybody.
This is for doctors and molecular biologists and
developmental biologists, everybody.
What should they know?"
And I said, "Rolf, your job is to figure out the
part from population genetics."
And he came back, after about two weeks,
and he said, "You know Steve,
I don't think there is anything."
I was shocked.
I said, "Rolf, you're a population geneticist.
This stuff is important, right?"
And then he said, "You know,
the way we normally teach population genetics,
which is as a big bunch of equations that are about drift
and frequency change under selection and so forth,
most people end up not really needing that.
What they need to know is that there are four main genetic
systems and that genetic change is slow, fast,
slow."
So that's where this lecture came from.
It came from somebody thinking deeply about that,
and asking lots of people.
Now if you like this, there's a whole field there,
there's a whole bunch of wonderful stuff that you can do.
But these are the things that everybody I think should know.
So here's the outline.
I'm going to give you the context, the historical context
that led to the concentration on genetics in evolutionary
biology.
I'll talk a little about the main genetic systems.
Then I'll run through changes in gene frequencies under
selection and, if I have time,
I'll get to selection on quantitative traits.
If I don't get to selection on quantitative traits,
it will be because I have engaged in a dialog with you
about some interesting puzzles, and that dialog is more
important to me than getting to quantitative genetics.
Okay?
So here's how genetics became a key element in evolutionary
thought.
Darwin did not have a plausible genetic mechanism and he failed
to read Mendel's paper, which came out six years after
he wrote The Origin, but before he constructed some
of the later editions of his book,
and so he reacted by incorporating elements of
Lamarck into his later editions.
If you read the Sixth Edition of The Origin of Species,
it's got some really Lamarckian statements in it,
inheritance of acquired characteristics.
Anybody here know what the problem was with Darwin's
original model?
Anybody know how Darwin thought genetics worked in 1859?
He had a model of blending inheritance.
That meant that he thought that when the gametes were formed,
gemmules from all over the body, that had been out there
soaking up information about the environment,
swam down into the gametes, into the gonads,
carrying with them information about the environment into the
gametes, and that then when the zygote
was formed, that the information from the
mother and the information from the father blended together like
two liquids.
In other words, he didn't think of genes as
distinct material particles.
He thought of them as fluids.
Now if I give you a glass of red wine and a glass of white
wine, and I pour them together, I get pink wine.
And if I take that glass of pink wine and I pour it together
with another glass of white wine, I get even lighter pink.
And you can see that if I continue this,
pretty soon red disappears completely.
The problem with blending inheritance is that the parental
condition gets blended out and there isn't really a
preservation of information.
That's why Darwin came under attack.
And Mendel wasn't known, and he resorted to
Lamarckianism, and he was wrong.
So genetics became an issue.
In the year 1900 there was a simultaneous rediscovery of
Mendel's Laws, and at that point people went
back and they read Mendel's paper,
and they realized that they had missed this 35 years earlier.
Then the so-called 'fly group' of Thomas Hunt Morgan and
Sturtevant and Bridges, who were working at Cal-Tech,
demonstrated that genes are carried on chromosomes.
And enough then was known about cytology,
so that we knew that chromosomes had an elaborate
kind of behavior, at mitosis and meiosis,
and people then, about 1915, showed that in fact
the behavior of chromosomes was consistent with Mendel's Laws.
They didn't know at that point what chromosomes were made out
of.
They had no notion of the genetic code,
but they could establish experimentally that genes were
on chromosomes; and that was done by 1915.
However, there were still issues about whether all of this
would actually work at the population level.
It was not immediately clear that you could take Mendelian
genetics and then construct populations out of it,
that obeyed Mendel's Laws, and have natural selection
work.
To do that actually required a fair amount of math,
and the people who did it were Ronald Fisher,
J.B.S. Haldane and Sewall Wright, and they did it between
about 1918 and 1932.
In so doing, they also invented much of what
is now regarded as basic statistics.
So Fisher had to invent analysis of variance in order to
understand quantitative genetics,
and Wright had to invent path coefficients in order to
understand how pedigrees translate into patterns of
inheritance.
So these guys laid the foundations.
As a result of that, genetics really became regarded
as kind of the core of evolutionary biology during the
twentieth century, and there's been a tremendous
concentration on it.
And it is still true that many people will not accept a claim
about any evolutionary process unless it can be shown to be
consistent with genetics.
That's sort of a Gold Standard.
If you can't do it genetically, if you can demonstrate it's
genetically illogical, then a claim just falls
theoretically; you don't even have to go out
and get the data.
Therefore, of course, the Young Turks have great joy
in discovering cases that don't fit and come up with epigenetics
and lots of stuff like that.
At any rate, that's ahead of you;
that's not today.
The genetic system of a species is really the basic determinant
of its rate of change.
So we have sexual versus asexual species--there are
complications to this--and we have haploid versus diploid,
and there are other ploidy levels.
Can anybody name me asexual vertebrates;
not sexual vertebrates but asexual vertebrates?
Anybody ever heard of an asexual vertebrate?
Fish, amphibians, reptiles, birds,
mammals?
Student: Wasn't there a recent documentation of a shark?
You mentioned it.
Prof: I could imagine that a shark might be capable of
being asexual.
I haven't heard of that case.
Student: I think it was kind of a
>
Prof: Yes, there are some.
There are some asexual lizards.
There are some interestingly asexual fish.
There are some frogs that manage to be kind of
quasi-asexual by using male sperm but then not incorporating
it into gametes-- excuse me, in the developing
baby.
So they use it just to stimulate development.
There's one case in captivity of an asexual turkey.
But asexual types are not frequent among vertebrates.
They are common in plants.
Of course, most bacterial sex is asexual, although bacteria do
have a bit of sex.
You're diploid; your adult large form is
diploid.
Anybody know what group of plants is haploid in the state
in which you normally see them in nature, where the big
recognizable thing is haploid?
I'll show you one in a minute.
I just wanted to check.
Mosses; mosses are haploid.
Okay, so this is what's going on with these four systems.
Basically the difference between sexual haploids and
sexual diploids is the point in the lifecycle where meiosis
occurs.
If the adult is diploid and meiosis occurs in gonads in the
adults that produce gametes, and then the zygote form
develops so that all of the cells in the developing organism
are diploid, you get the diploid cycle.
If you have the zygote having meiosis immediately,
or shortly after being formed, so that the developing young
are haploid, then you get a haploid adult.
So this is what moss do and this is what we do.
Then we have asexual haploids and asexual diploids,
and at least in outline they look pretty simple.
Asexual diploid, just makes a copy of itself;
just goes through mitosis, makes babies.
Asexual haploid, same kind of thing.
So those are the four major genetic systems.
There are many, many variations on them.
So the asexual haploids are things like the tuberculosis
pathogen, blue-green algae,
the bread mould, the penicillin fungus,
cellular slime moulds, and they constitute the bulk of
the organisms on the planet.
Sexual haploids are things like moss, and red algae;
most fungi are sexual haploids.
In this case you can see that's where the haploid adult is in
the lifecycle.
There are where the gametes are formed.
They are formed up on the head of the adult.
You can see the pink and the blue are coding for the male and
the female gametes, on different parts of the
gametophyte.
Then the zygote forms where the sperm gets into an ovule,
on the tip of the plant, and then the young actually
develop up here.
So this is haploid up here and then the spores go out--meiosis
has occurred in here and the spores go out as haploid spores.
So that is a sexual haploid lifecycle.
The asexual diploids include the dynoflagelates;
there are about ten groups of the protoctists--
that's the modern name for what you think of as protozoa,
but it also includes some single-celled organisms that
have chloroplasts in them-- the unicellular algae,
some protozoa, some unicellular fungi.
There are a lot of multi-cellular animals that are
asexual diploids, and this one here,
the bdelloid rotifer is one of them.
It is called a scandalous ancient asexual.
Anybody know why the word 'scandalous' is used in this
context?
Yes?
What?
Student: No males.
Prof: There are no males; bdelloid rotifers do not have
any males, nobody's ever seen a male bdelloid rotifer.
But that's not the scandal; I mean, if you're a male you
might think it was scandalous.
Right?
>
But for an evolutionary biologist, no,
that's not scandalous.
Well it actually has to do with this part of it right here.
Almost all asexual organisms on the planet,
that are multi-cellular--leaving out the
bacteria-- but all the multi-cellular ones
are derived from sexual ancestors and originated
relatively recently, with a few exceptions,
and this is one of the exceptions.
There is a whole huge body of literature on the evolution of
sex that says one of the things that sex is good for is that it
allows long persistence.
We see that sexual things have been in a sexual state on the
Tree of Life for a long time, and the asexual things have
branched off of it, and we don't see very many
ancient ones.
The reason for that--we'll come to that,
when we get to the evolution of sex--
is that both because of mutations and because of
pathogens, sex repairs damage and defends
the organism against attack.
So this is a low maintenance, poorly defended organism,
and it looks like it's been around without sex for perhaps
300 million years.
The scandal is we don't know how it did it.
Okay?
That's why it's called a scandalous ancient asexual.
Yes, that's a very intellectual definition of scandal;
I agree.
Okay, sexual diploids.
You guys are sexual diploids, this bee is a sexual diploid,
and that flower is a sexual diploid.
They have this kind of lifecycle, as is sketched here,
the one that I talked about earlier.
So about twenty animal phyla are sexual diploids.
Many plants, most multi-cellular plants are,
and there are some algae protozoa and fungi that are
sexual diploids.
They include the malaria and sleeping sickness pathogens.
There are some things that don't fit;
the sexual diploid part doesn't fit, for malaria and sleeping
sickness.
The things that are alternating between being haploid and
diploid, with neither one dominating,
are mushrooms, microsporidian parasites,
which are things that are actually quite common in many
insects, and the malaria--malaria has a
very complex lifecycle.
So it is haploid inside your red blood cells,
it's diploid at a certain point in a mosquito,
and it's moving back and forth.
The things that alternate sexual and asexual reproduction:
there are some rotifers, some cnidarians,
some water fleas, some annelids.
There's a great little annelid that lives in the bottom of the
Harbor of Naples in Italy, and it actually does
everything.
It can be asexual--the same species--it can be asexual;
it can be born as a female and turn into a male;
it can be born as a male and turn into a female;
and it can be born as both and do both.
So some things are really flexible, but most things
aren't.
And the timing of sexuality and asexuality is an important part
of the lifecycle of all of these things.
Last fall, for example, there were huge jellyfish
blooms over much of the world's oceans,
and that's part of a complex lifecycle in which there is an
asexual phase on the floor of the ocean,
that builds up what looks like a stack of dinner plates,
and then the top plate flips off and turns into a jellyfish.
It goes off as a jellyfish and has sex and makes larvae,
and then goes down and turns into an asexual thing on the
bottom that makes stacks of dinner plates.
So there's a lot of variety out there.
All of these things probably evolved from an asexual haploid;
and we say that because we believe that the bacterial state
was the ancestral condition.
Okay, now genetics constrains evolution,
and genetics is doing something to evolutionary thought which is
about what chemistry does to metabolism and structure,
and is about what physics does to chemistry.
Okay?
There's a broad analogy there.
If you want to understand molecular and cell biology,
you learn a lot of chemistry.
If you want to understand some evolution, then you need to
learn a little bit about how genetics constrains evolution,
and so you need a little math.
So I'm going to give you some simple math, and here's some
terminology to soak up.
So we're going to represent these ideas by symbols.
We're going to call alleles Aa.
So those are two alleles at one locus;
a little exercise of genetic terminology.
We're going to let p be the frequency of A1,
and q the frequency of A2.
And frequency just means the following: some traits are
Mendelian, which means that they're easily recognized in the
phenotype.
One of the Mendelian traits in humans is the ability to curl
your tongue.
I am a tongue curler.
Okay?
How many of you can curl your tongues?
Okay, let's say it's about 45.
How many of you cannot curl your tongues?
Let's say it's about 30.
So the frequency of tongue curling is going to be--I'm just
making up the numbers, right?--45 divided by 75.
That's how we get the number.
And by the way, the frequency of the other one
is going to be 1 minus that frequency, because p plus q is
equal to 1; and we'll let s be the
selection coefficient, which is measuring the
reproductive success of the organism carrying this trait,
the difference that it makes.
And if we look at the genetic change in asexual haploids,
basically what one does is make a table of the process;
and it is moving from young, in the present generation,
through the adult stage, to young in the next
generation.
So we try to go through one generation.
This is an active Cartesian reduction.
We're taking a complex process and breaking it down into the
parts that are essential for the thing that we're thinking about.
We have genotype frequencies--for genotypes A1
and A2 they're p and q--and we have relative fitnesses up here.
The only place that selection is making any difference,
on this whole page that's in front of you,
is right here.
And basically what--our placing that there is an act that means
the following.
We are only going to think about the case in which there is
some difference in the juvenile survival of A2;
it's different from A1.
If it makes it to adulthood, there's no difference;
we don't put that down in the table.
So this is a case where we're just--you know,
it's a special case--we're just looking at the juvenile survival
difference between A1 and A2.
What happens?
Well it changes the frequencies of A1 and A2 in the adults.
Basically it changes them by reducing the number of A2s.
Some of them have died out; that's 1 minus s,
that's what the 1 minus s is doing.
You can take these expressions here and you can simplify them
so they look like this-- it's just a little bit of
algebra--and because these are the frequencies in the adults,
the young in the next generation have exactly those
frequencies, because there is no selective
difference in the adult stage.
Okay?
That's what that table means.
Now a little bit about this.
This little process that I've gone through,
which probably looks like remarkably simplistic
bookkeeping to you, is actually the part of doing
applied mathematics which is the most difficult.
It is the translation of a process into something
analytically simple, that you can deal with.
In the act of doing it, you make certain assumptions to
simplify the situation, and by writing them down it
helps you to remember what assumptions you made and what
thing you're actually looking at.
We're not looking at all of evolution here,
we're looking at a very special case;
we're looking at asexual haploids where selective
differences only occur in juveniles.
What happens is you get a change in the gene frequencies
of the adults that result from that process,
and then that exact change is passed on to the next
generation.
So that's the part of this process that I want you to
remember.
You can go look this stuff up any time.
You don't need to memorize that.
You can program this as recursion equations and apply
them repeatedly.
Okay?
Now let's do it for sexual diploids.
In the sexual diploids, you've already been exposed to
the Hardy-Weinberg Law, this p^(2) 2pq q^(2) law.
In order to get it, we have to assume random mating
in a big population.
The reason you need the big population is so that those p's
and q's are actually accurate measures.
In a small population they're noisy, but in a big population
they are good stable estimates.
And if there's random mating, that means that matings are
occurring in proportion to the frequency of each type.
So you get a Punnet diagram like this.
You have the probability of one of these alleles occurring;
and one parent is going to p, the other allele in that parent
q.
Same for the other parent.
These are the possible zygotes that will result from that.
This one has probability p^(2; )this one has probability q^(2);
and these two together have probability 2pq.
That's just simple basic probability theory.
Now, the important thing about the Hardy-Weinberg Law is that
it implies that there's no change from one generation to
the next.
The gene frequencies under Hardy-Weinberg don't change.
That means that the information that's been accumulated on what
works in the population doesn't change for random reasons.
If it's going to change, it's going to change because
that big population is going to come under selection.
Okay?
That means that replication is accurate and fair,
at the level of the population, just as it is at the level of
the cell.
Now, of course, gene drift is going on,
but we're not so worried here about gene drift,
because gene drift is affecting things that aren't making a
difference to selection, and we're building models of
selection.
What Hardy-Weinberg does is tell you if there isn't any
drift, if there isn't any mutation,
if there isn't any selection, if there isn't any migration in
the population, and if you don't have a high
mutation rate, things are going to stay the
same.
So if they're changing, one of those things is making a
difference.
Okay?
And that gives us a baseline.
So it gives us a baseline to see the process of selection
occurring, but it also means that random
mating in large populations preserves information on what
worked in the past.
So you don't have to invent everything all over again.
And a note for future lectures, these are also the conditions
that remove conflict by guaranteeing fairness.
So basically the Hardy-Weinberg situation is one in which
everything that was in the population last generation has
exactly the same chance of getting into the next
generation, in proportion to its frequency;
nothing is going to change.
Okay, here's a genetic counseling problem,
and I'm going to take a little time on this.
We go back to John and Jill.
They've fallen in love, they want to get married,
but they're worried.
John's brother died of a genetic disease,
and that is a nasty one.
It's recessive, it's lethal,
it kills anybody that carries it before they can reproduce.
That's fact one.
Jill doesn't have any special history of this disease in her
family, but that history's not well
known, and so we estimate the probability that Jill carries
the disease from the frequency of deaths in the general
population, and that frequency is 1%;
to make it easier for you to calculate.
Okay?
What's the probability that they will have a child that dies
from this disease in childhood?
The probability is .03.
Your problem is not to tell me .03, your problem is to tell me
why did I use that equation?
Okay?
So take a look at that equation for a minute,
take a look at that problem, and let's go through and pull
it apart.
Can anybody see why either the two-thirds or the one-quarter is
in the equation?
Student: We know that his brother has a recessive
version of the lethal gene, and therefore John is either
heterozygous-- doesn't look like it's dominant,
looks like it's recessive.
So if he is heterozygous or homozygous recessive,
then he's carrying the gene; which is what we're worried
about.
So there's a two-thirds chance that he is either carrying it or
actually has the disease.
Prof: That's correct.
The only slip you made in expressing that is that we know
that if they are going to have a child that has the defect,
they both must be heterozygous, and so we're concentrating
specifically on what's the probability that they're
heterozygous.
You then gave me that probability.
Does anybody have a problem seeing why the probability that
John is a heterozygote is two-thirds, rather than 50%;
excuse me, that the baby is a heterozygote is two-thirds?
Yes?
Student: So we're going to keep him as a
>.
Prof: Yes, you do.
Okay.
This is for the baby.
Okay?
If John is a heterozygote and if Jill is a heterozygote,
they can have either a homozygous recessive,
and that one will die before birth;
they can have a homozygous dominant, perfectly healthy;
or they can have a heterozygote.
The probability of the homozygote recessive is 25%,
the probability of the homozygous dominant is 25%,
and the probability of the heterozygote is 50%.
But, the probability that John and Jill will have a baby that
dies from this disease in childhood is going to be
therefore this one-quarter.
This two-thirds is going to be the probability that John is a
heterozygote.
How do we know that John--John's parents were both
heterozygotes?
Student: They had a recessive son.
Prof: They had a recessive son.
John's parents had to be heterozygotes.
Therefore, given that John's parents were heterozygotes,
his probability is two-thirds.
We know he survived to adulthood;
the other 25% died.
So of those who survived to adulthood, two-thirds are
heterozygotes and one-third are homozygotes.
Student: Why can't one be homozygote recessive and the
other one be heterozygous?
>.
Prof: Because if one, the parent--if one parent was a
homozygote, it could only have been
homozygous dominant, because it survived to
adulthood, to have a child.
And if the other parent was a heterozygote,
the only possibilities for the children are both heterozygotes;
and that wasn't the case, because John's brother died.
Okay?
So this is the probability that John is a heterozygote.
This is the probability that if John and Jill have a baby,
it will have the problem.
What's this thing in the middle--2 times 0.9 times 0.1?
Student: >
Prof: Right.
That's the probability that Jill is a heterozygote,
and we get that from here.
The square root of 1% is .1.
1 minus .1 is .9.
This is q and this is p and this is 2pq.
Okay?
Where did we get this from?
That's in Jill's part of the population.
Those are the baby--oh you've got it.
Student: The probability has to be out of the
entire population, and the long-term population,
they can't reproduce-- >.
Prof: Right.
So we have to correct the percentages for the ones that
have died.
Yes, you got it.
Do you see how much goes into dissecting an equation like
that?
But because we've set up the logical apparatus,
we can go through a sequence of steps and say,
"Okay, first we know they both have to be heterozygous.
Then, if they are both heterozygous,
the probability that Jack is, is two-thirds;
the probability that Jill is, is 2pq, corrected for the fact
that 1% have died.
She has survived, so we have to correct for that.
Then this is the probability that their baby has the
disease."
That's the kind of process that one goes through when thinking
about population genetics.
This is the table for sexual diploids that reflects this kind
of thinking.
It is more complicated because now we have to keep track of
both the haploid and the diploid condition.
So we have these haploid gametes, with frequencies p and
q.
We have the diploid zygotes.
Then another process comes in.
We can have a selective difference--I made a S here;
I made a -S in the last one.
I made that change deliberately,
just so that you'd see it as arbitrary;
because we can make S negative or positive itself.
Right?
S doesn't have to be a positive number;
neither does H.
Anybody have an idea what H might be in there for?
It's in there to represent something that's going on in
genetics.
Yes?
Student: Is it heritability?
Prof: No it's not heritability,
in this context.
Okay?
Yes?
Student: Is it the Marsh's coefficient for being
heterozygous?
>
Prof: Not in this context.
Good idea, but no.
What is it about that heterozygote that doesn't
necessarily have anything to do with selection?
H expresses dominance.
It expresses the degree to which A1 is covering up A2 in
the phenotype.
Dominance itself is not something that's always there.
If there isn't any dominance, then the heterozygote is just
exactly halfway in the phenotype between the two homozygotes.
So H is a little mathematical symbol that allows us to deal
with situations in which either there's a lot of dominance or
none at all.
If H = 0, there's no dominance.
Okay?
No excuse me, the way it's set up,
if H = 0, then A1, A2 is just exactly like A1,
A1, and there is dominance.
So we have to make H something non-zero, in order to express
deviations from dominance, the way this one is set up.
At any rate, the--what's going on here is
essentially the same kind of selection process.
There is a selective difference, which is
disadvantaging A2.
So A2 doesn't survive as well as A1.
When it is in the heterozygous form, it may do better,
if there's some dominance.
And that results in a more complicated set of equations.
W here is defined as this big term.
We have basically the adults being p^(2), 2 pq times 1 plus
hs.
And A2, A2 has a frequency of q^(2) times 1 plus s,
which is the selection coefficient over here.
So q is changing the most, and to the degree that A2 can
be seen in the heterozygote, it will also be affected by s,
but it won't be affected if there is complete dominance.
Okay?
So if h is zero, there's no effect of selection
on the heterozygote; this term cancels out.
The result of that is that you get these frequencies forming
the next generations.
Now there a couple of ways of setting up this whole
derivation, and in the Second Edition of
the book, Box 4.1 and Box 4.2 do it a
little bit differently.
You might want to just step through those things in section.
The goal here is not to memorize how to derive the
equations, or to memorize the equations.
Because, as I've said, you can always pick them up in
a book, or pull them off the web, and you can find programs
that will do it all the time.
The goal is to understand what it is that population
geneticists are thinking about when they set it up this way,
and what power it gives them.
So let me just show you what happens when you program these
recursion equations.
By the way, they're called recursion equations because they
give us the frequency in the next generation as a function of
the frequency in this generation.
So they form kind of a Markov chain.
They allow us to calculate next time from this time;
that's something computers are really good at.
So this is the take-home message of all that analysis:
you look at genetic change, in asexual haploids,
sexual diploids, and it's slow at the beginning,
fast in the middle; it's slow at the end.
The haploids change faster than the diploids,
and the dominants change faster than the recessives.
So let's step through that and see if you can tell me why this
is the case.
First let's take the asexual haploids, or haploids of any
kind.
Why is it that haploids change gene frequencies faster,
for given selection pressures, than do diploids?
Yes?
Student: The entire gene--all the genes are
inherited.
It's not all >;
it's sort of a complete replication of them,
the order.
Prof: Well that is what a haploid is,
but that doesn't explain why it's faster.
The statement is true, but it's not an answer to my
question.
Another try.
Yes?
Student: Well all the >
, the bad genes die off.
>
Prof: Okay, that's going in the right
direction, but I think it can be expressed even more clearly.
Yes?
Student: >
Prof: That's interesting.
That actually gets into the evolution of sex.
I'm actually thinking though about an answer that has more to
do with developmental biology and not so much to do with sex,
at this point.
Um, actually I think that, uh, your answer is partially
correct, but it's more complicated than what I was
looking for.
>
Yes?
Student: Is it that all asexuals can reproduce?
Prof: No, it's not that all of the
asexuals can reproduce.
Many of them die as juveniles.
It has to do with haploidy versus diploidy.
Yes?
Student: Then if the organism has the allele that's
different, it's going to best.
Prof: Yes.
Student: And that's when this other comes along.
Prof: Every gene is expressed, and there's no
dominance covering up any hidden genetic information.
The genes are exposed to selection, in haploids.
Yes?
Student: So why is that faster than a dominant zygote,
>?
Prof: Good.
We'll find out as we go through the next questions.
Okay?
So the haploids are faster than a dominant diploid because--?
Student: >.
That's why it's a recessive gene.
Prof: Basically, yes.
The heterozygotes react like the dominant,
but contain the recessive.
And so if you're measuring the rate of evolution as the rate at
which the dominant takes over the population,
it's carrying along in the heterozygotes a bunch of
recessives.
Okay?
They're doing just as well as it is.
So development, which is covering up the
difference between the two, is actually giving the
recessives an advantage and slowing down the rate at which
the dominant can take over.
Okay?
Recessive diploid; I think that you now see why
that would be the slowest.
If we have an advantageous recessive gene,
it gets slowed down by the fact that when it's in the
heterozygote, its effects are being covered
up by the other allele.
Okay, why is it S-shaped?
Why is the trait--let's do it for a dominant diploid sexual.
Okay?
Slow at the beginning, fast in the middle,
really slow at the end.
Let's concentrate on first why this is really slow at the end,
and then we can also look at why a recessive diploid sexual
is really slow at the beginning.
What do you have to think about in order to pull the answer out
of that diagram?
What proportion of the population is in heterozygous
form, as you get near the end?
If you're a dominant diploid sexual and you're at a frequency
of .9,81% of you are going to be dominant homozygotes;
18% of you are going to be heterozygotes;
and 1% of you are going to be recessive.
There are eighteen times as many heterozygotes as there are
recessive homozygotes.
Selection, at that point, is trying to eliminate that 1%
of recessive homozygotes.
It can't touch the 18%.
If you carry that process over, where we're dealing with .01
and .99, it gets even more extreme.
A tinier and tinier fraction of that population is a recessive
homozygote.
A larger and larger fraction of the remaining recessive alleles
are tied up in heterozygotes, where selection can't operate.
So this thing just slows way down.
It gets harder and harder to get rid of the disadvantageous
alleles, because a larger and larger
proportion of them-- not an absolute number but a
larger proportion of them-- are hidden in the heterozygotes.
The same thinking describes why evolutionary change in a
recessive diploid, where the recessive gene has
the advantage, is very slow at the beginning.
If a new recessive mutation comes into the population,
it's a very low frequency.
Its frequency is 1 divided by the number of individuals in the
population.
The only things it can mate with are dominant forms.
All of its babies are heterozygotes.
So at the beginning selection can't operate on it at all.
Only after two heterozygotes manage to get together and mate,
which means they must have come to fairly high frequency,
will they have a baby that is a recessive homozygote that
selection will operate on.
So it takes awhile to get this going.
And because of dominance, it takes a long time to build
up to the point where it accelerates.
But then at the end it's fast, because at the end the thing
that's being selected is the recessive, and it speeds up as
it goes through.
Okay, I thought this would happen,
uh, it's time for class to end, and I'm just getting to
quantitative genetics, and so I'm going to let you
pick up quantitative genetics from the lecture notes and from
the reading.
I do want to indicate as potential paper topics though
that quantitative genetics has got some of the most interesting
questions that we encounter in evolutionary biology,
and that it includes questions like the heritability of
intelligence, the heritability of SAT
scores--those are all things where the apparatus you need to
analyze the issue is given to you by quantitative genetics.
And there is a good paper on this, and I have put it up on
the course website, under Recommended Readings;
there's now a folder called Recommended Readings,
PDFs of Recommended Readings.
You can find this paper and some other ones in there,
if that's something that strikes your fancy.
Go take a look at the title and abstract.
So this is the summary of today's lecture.
And the next time we're going to talk about the origin and the
maintenance of genetic variation.
Mobile Analytics